Preliminary
report on surveying the need and developing the method
AJAI
R. SINGHl V.N.
BAGADIA2 P.V.
PRADHAN3 V.N.
ACHARYA4 SUMMARY
Psychology of deaths due to acute medical emergencies is under-researched.
Most studies till now have concentrated on extended-death situations like malignancy. This open pilot study of twenty-five
patients examines the psychological state of patients during a life threatening acute medical illness (Group A, ten patients)
and of those who survive such an experience (Group B, fifteen patients). The study finds psychological exploration both possible
and necessary if carried out in a discreet manner. Salient features of the interview technique are discussed. The study finds
out whether patients are aware of the possibility of terminality. The psychological disturbances manifest and nature of care
expected are also discussed. Near Death Experiences of those who acknowledge their occurrence are reported. Some nuances of
thanatological research are high-lighted: What are the abilities needed in an interviewer? Can such exploration increase psychological
distress in a patient already prone to it because of serious medical sickness? What impact such research can have on the interviewer
himself? The paper answers some of these common questions while developing the method of thanatological study in acute medical
death-situations. ………………………………………………………………….. 1.
Senior
Research Fellow, W.H.O. Collaborating Center in psychopharmacology 2.
Prof. Emeritus in
psychiatry, Seth G.S. Medical College and K.E.M. Hospital. Head, W.H.O. Collaborating Centre in Psychopharmacology. Department
of Psychiatry, K.E.M. Hospital 3. Hon.
Associate Prof. of Psychiatry. Seth GS Medical College and K.E.M. Hospital. Bombay
4.
Head, Department
of Medicine & Nephrology, and Prof. of Medicine. Seth G.S. Medical
College and K.E.M. Hospital. Bombay *Bombay Psychiatric Society Silver Jubilee Year Award
winning paper. Jan. 1988, at XXXX Annual Conference of IPS. Also President Award winning paper at XVIIl Annual Conference
of IPS. West Zone, Sept. 1987. Introduction Scientific enquiry into death is a relatively recent phenomenon. The psychiatrist's role in
the terminal patient traces its history to the last three quarters of a century. Treatment of the dying keeping his psychological needs in mind gained attention (HacKen and Weisman 1962) as did Hospice care (Krant 1981). Use of treatment
modalities in terminal patients like psychotherapy (Leshan and Leshan 1961, Cramond 1970, Stedeford 1979), Behaviour
Therapy (Whitman and Lukes 1975) and psychopharmacologic agents (Goldberg et al. 1973) were studied. Counter-transference
attitudes of health personnel dealing with the dying (Joseph 1962, Hicks and Daniels 1968) as well as when, what and how to
disclose (AitkenSwan and Easson 1959; Oken 1961, Weisman 1967) occupied the attention of other researches. Kubler-Ross’ epoch making work (1969,1974,1975) served as a launching pad, as though, for a
global upsurge in thanotological research. Near Death Experiences or NDEs also occupied the attention of quite a few
others (Moody 1975, Sabom 1982). Most of the studies (a number of them cannot be quoted for exigencies of space) have
evaluated dying as a process extended over a period of time, for example, following the diagnosis of malignancy through
the resultant morbidity to eventual death. Deaths due to acute life threatening medical emergencies has escaped
the attention of researchers, barring a few, like Levinson (1972) who discussed psychological state of patients facing sudden
death, Hackett & Cassem (1972) who examined the psychology of cardiac arrest,
and Bruhn et al. (1974) who evaluated psychological predictors of sudden death in cardiac patients. This, however, is a miniscule
minority, probably for pragmatic reasons. The equation needs to be set right. Moreover death and dying has escaped
the attention of researchers in India, in this age of the thanatologic imperative (Kothari and Mehta 1986) and in a
country which not without reason can boast of a great philosophical tradition on the subject, right from the Vedas to
Sri Aurobindo (Kamath 1978). The present effort may be considered one in the direction of setting the equation right.
Aims
of the Study 1. Is
it possible to study the psychology of death and dying in a general medical set-up? 2. Whether there is a
need to do so? 3. How does one go about
it? 4. Are psychiatric disturbances
manifest by such patients? 5. Are Near Death Experiences
(NDEs) reported in those who recover? 6. What are the needs
and expectations of such patients? Material
and Methods A total of 25 patients (Group A, 10, and, Group B, 15) from the General Medical Ward administering both
emergency and ongoing care in a teaching general hospital in Greater Bombay were included in this open pilot study. Their
ages ranged from 12 to 61 years. The sample was made up predominantly by the age groups 16-30 years (56%) and 31-45 years
(28%). Group A (10 patients - 7 male and,
3 female) was seen when acutely sick medically and emergency life-sustaining procedures were being administered.
Group A thus consists of seriously sick patients with a definite mortality risk. Group B (15 patients - 9 male, 6 female)
consisted of patients who had been through a life threatening medical illness and were now in the recovery phase. They came
from either the ICCU or Intensive Nephrology Care Unit. Interview
Technique All patients were first screened on the weekly rounds with the Physician-In-Charge and the resident staff.
They were then interviewed by one of the authors having post-graduate qualification in psychiatry (ARS). The Mental Status
Examination of all patients was done. The interview schedule was semi-structured, being conducted in 1-6 sessions. Open-ended
questions were usually asked. Rarely were leading questions used because of their psychologically disturbing potential.
This, rather than asking 'Do you think your sickness can result in something serious? May be...death?' the question put was,
'What do you feel can be the outcome of your present sickness?' If the question was parried, one or two discreet questions put were, 'You know, being in a hospital with
lots of doctors and nurses around and so many tubes being used on a person can cause anxiety. How does that affect you?'
If still there was no answer, a comment was made like, 'It appears you are satisfied with what is being done for you. We are
happy you are confident of a smooth recovery?' From either of the comments, the patient broke his initial reserve and
became communicative. The interview itself had to be discreet because of the sensitive nature of the subject
tackled. Also there was the continuous need to avoid arousing undue anxiety in patients, already over-burdened with the distress
consequent to physical morbidity, and even in relatives, and the ward-staff. No misplaced zeal need be used in any psychological
investigation that adversely affects the emotional equilibrium of the seriously sick and their relatives. A firm commitment
to this effect from the psychiatric side is a must, whether asked for or not. This tends to keep over-enthusiasm in check
and zealousness tempered, while the genuine keenness of research is never compromised. Results
and Discussion I. The Sample: Most Group A patients developed their sickness less than three months
prior to hospitalization (60%) while Group B was represented, 73 %, by those who developed their sickness more than three months prior to hospitalization. Group A, thus, represented patients who had fallen
sick relatively recently (average duration 10 days) while Group B consisted of those who had symptoms for a longer period
(average duration 100 days). Majority of Group A patients were seen 24 hours - 14 days of developing acute symptoms
(80%). Group B patients were seen 8 days - 30 days following recovery of the acute episode (87%). Thus the acutely sick were seen during their acute phase and the recovering patient in a reasonable
time following the acute phase and during their active recovery and rehabilitative period. The sample may, therefore,
be considered fairly representative of the phenomenon studied. Table 1 Diagnostic Break-up, Group A (N = 10)
Diagnosis Expired Recovered Total Fulminant
Pneumonia 2 1 3 Advanced
Tuberculosis 1 1 2 Hepatic
Encephalopathy 2 0 2 Acute
Rental failure 1 2 3 6 4 10 Table 2 Diagnostic Break-up, Group B (N = 15)
Comment: All patients needed emergency resuscitation measures and had
survived. II. Diagnosis: Table 1 gives the diagnostic break-up in Group A. The common life threatening
sicknesses were Fulminant Pneumonia, Advanced Tuberculosis, Hepatic Encephalopathy and Acute Renal Failure. Six of these patients
expired and four recovered. Table 2 shows the diagnostic break-up in Group B. Myocardial Infarction and/or Ischaemia, and Acute Renal
Failure in patients with Chronic Renal Failure are represented here. The diagnoses represent acute life threatening medical
situations commonly encountered in a general medical ward. III. Mental
Status Examination Table 1 Group A: Mental Status Examination (N = 10)
Note: More than one symptom was often present in the same patient. Table 3 shows Mental Status Examination (MSE) results in Group A. Clouding of consciousness was present in all patients
at some time or other. But this did not preclude further MSE. 50% went with coma and 60% had episodes of delirium, either
preceding or succeeding the MSE. Lack of communicativeness (3 pts.), lack of personal hygiene (4 pts.) and incontinence
(2 pts.) were the usual accompaniments of disturbed consciousness. Terror with florid paranoid ideation (3 pts.); hallucinations (visual 2 pts; auditory 1 pt.); depressed
mood (2 pts.); crying spells, guilt feelings, suicidal ideation (1 pt.); anxiety (4 pts.); somatic preoccupation (5 pts.);
conversion symptoms (1 pt.); and compulsive symptoms (1 pt.) were also seen. Orientation to time, place, or person was disturbed in seven out of ten patients. Memory appeared disturbed
in five, immediate (5 pts.), recent (5 pts.) and past (2 pts.). Intelligence was affected in two patients. Anxiety, Paranoid ideation, Depressed mood, Conversion symptoms and Obsessive compulsive symptoms
were present and seemed to be exacerbation of basic personality characteristics. Group A, thus, consisted of acute medical emergencies who progressively lapsed into disorganized states
of the mind. This is especially true of those who subsequently expired (60%). These patients were reasonably young and
the duration of morbidity was quite short. Thus, acute medical emergencies may leave little time for psychological understanding
or for the relatives to cope with the probability of death in a family. It differs considerably from an extended
death situation like malignancy. Follow up of close relatives of such patients needs further study. Important areas could
be the repercussions of lack of time for psychological rehabilitation, or the time to say good-bye. Also worth exploration
would be how patients and their relatives compress the stages of dying in the short time at their disposal, how they succeed
and where they fail, and the squeal of either. Table 4 shows mental status examination in Group B. Almost all patients were eager to establish communication.
Only 3 patients showed mild clouding of consciousness. No perceptual or cognitive disturbances were noted; neither
were Insight or judgment impaired. Table 4 Group B: Mental Status Examination (N = 15)
Note: Symptoms overlap in some patients. Anxiety symptoms were manifest m eight patients of Group B. They were apprehensive about the sickness,
were afraid it was severe and would seriously cripple their day to day activities (although the medical opinion warranted
such a conclusion in only four such cases). They complained of difficulty in getting sleep (8 pts.), tremulousness
(7pts.), palpitations (6 pts.), and fear of fresh acute episode of their sickness.
They occasionally showed clinging behaviour (4 pts.) and asked for repeated reassurance from medical staff and relatives (6
pts.). They had occasional spell of crying (3 pts.) with sulking behaviour (4 pts.) and irritability (6 pts.). Some complained
of fleeting suicidal thought (2 pts.) All patients were anxious about the future welfare of their family members. Three patients showed depressed mood. They felt life was not worth living (3 pts.), it would be better
to die (3 pts.), and after-life would be better (2 pts.). They felt guilty of insufficient provision for family members (2pts.),
prayed for a miracle to cure their sickness (2 pts.), prayed that life be prolonged to carry out pressing business and family
duties (2 pts.). Two patients appeared indifferent to the severity of their sickness. They maintained a resigned attitude,
appeared to take excessive interest in religion and had faith of tiding over their present crisis. They however also complained
of petty inconsistencies in the behaviour of relatives and medical staff. Two patients appeared to take the experience
with minimal of psychological symptoms. Group B patients showed a keen desire to talk and unburden themselves, but this need was not met by either the staff or the relatives. IV. Psychiatric Disturbances: Anxiety was the predominant symptom in both Groups A and B (Table
3 and 4). This is understandable due to an experience which is felt so near death. Lot of this anxiety centered around
the future course and outcome of the illness which almost no patient knew for sure. This is probably due to an honest doubt
in the physicians' mind whether such knowledge can have its own psychological repercussions. Anxiety (53%) and Depressive
(20%) were seen as clinically diagnosable disease entities in Group B patients (Table 3). Terror with paranoid ideation where manifest (Table 3) involved persecutory delusions about relatives
and medical staff. Those patients who showed no abnormality (Table 4) seemed to have both an element of faith and awareness
of practicalities. They had had fleeting anxiety and depressive symptoms. They felt grateful for the support of
at least one significant relative, of religious scriptures, and the ward-staff in their crisis. V. Awareness of Terminality: 60% patients in both Group A and B were aware of the possibility of
terminality (Table 5). The similarity in both groups is striking and needs further study. As at present, it appeared
more a manifestation of sickness severity rather than a premonition. Table 5 Aware of Possibility of Terminality in both Groups A and B (N = 25)
Note :- 60% of both Groups A and B were aware of the possibility of terminality. VI. Near Death Experience NDE): Table 6 shows NDEs. 10 patients out of 19 replied in the affirmative when questioned
for it. Their experiences are mainly ‘Intense Darkness’ (3 patients) and ‘Total Silence’ (3 patients).
Other items were bright lights, receding into the background, vision of Christ and vision of Yama. Table 6 Near Death Experience (NDE) (N=10, Group A-2, Group B 8)
*Denotes irrelevance of % statistics here because of small sample. VII. Nature of Care Asked for: Almost all patients asked for prompt relief from pain and discomfort (84%) and distress
free digestive and evacuative functioning (84%). They wanted reassurance that they would improve (60%) and asked for a painless
end if it became inevitable (36%). They liked cheerfulness in medical staff (80%), appreciated competence in symptom relief
(88%) and singled out individual staff members for accolades (7%) and brick bats (56%). They disapproved of anger in staff
members (72%), neglect (52%), evasive answers (405) or false reassurances (32%). These items show that patients were keenly
aware of what when on in their environment. It was not always that detailed talk or reassurance was asked for. Often patients appreciated acts like a warm greeting,
a gentle touch of comfort, a helping hand in getting up from bed, an allowing of the doctor's hand to be clasped, and an opportunity
to cry or touch the feet. A regular, brief, cheerful visit was better appreciated than a prolonged, irregular one. Patients
disliked caring persons who avoided eye contact either while talking to them or even while passing them by. In most cases that survived, patients were
grateful to recount the heroic measures carried out. But many were equally expectant that care of a significant intensity
would still be continued. A word or two about patients' overzealous methods at rehabilitation also reveals another aspect
of the nature of care these patients need. This applies particularly to patients physically restricted during their
rehabilitation, specially the myocardial infarction patients of Group B. While on Occupational Therapy, they sometimes over-stretched
themselves when unobserved. Sexual needs were again a neglected area of counseling. At/east two patients of infarction
confided they masturbated in hospital without knowledge of the medical staff. They were too embarrassed to ask for advice.
A straight forward approach to counseling about sex or masturbation in recuperating infarct patients is therefore/ necessary.
Although the number is small (i.e. 3 out of 9 cardiac patients), these are sensitive areas. Patients may be unable to reveal
them, yet it may he the need of quite a few. Further study of factors which help and/or hinder rehabilitation and how the
caring team could by simple and straight forward explanation help in dispelling doubts and fears would be worthwhile. VIII. Comments on the Interview Session & Techniques:
The authors found that initial resistance to being interviewed was born out of a great need to deny. Any denial, by both patients and relatives, seems to serve an importance social function (Beilen, 1981).
It maintains the semblance of a relationship based on hope and courage. Where denial was an important defense, the interview
was conducted in short sessions, in as less glum a manner as possible. The manner of the interview was such as to instil
cordiality. It involved, amongst other things, listening to the difficulties of being in a general medical ward.
It also meant passing on relevant information to the ward staff for necessary action. The interviewer thus became something
of a liaison worker here. All of us have a rather strong inner resistance to letting dying persons say
what's on their minds (Cassem and Stewart 1975), because it increases our own death-anxiety in turn. This applies to medical
men as well as relatives. But practically every empirical study in this field has emphasised the ability to listen over
the ability to say something (ibid). The patient does not only want answers to his questions. He often wants a receptive,
knowing and caring situation. In the bargain, he is ready to offer the researchers any number of enlightening answers
to his questions. Probably, Saunders summed it up beautifully when she said,
"The real question is not, 'What do you tell your patients?' but rather, 'what do you let your patients tell you?" (Saunders 1969). It is difficult to resist the experience of being carried away by certain emotions
in thanatological research. This is the universal experience of most such researchers. This again is a reality every
researcher in this field must be ready to accept. In fact if he does not do so, it is only a measure of his denial. or lack
of awareness. As Kubler-Ross (1975) so succinctly put it. "Each encounter with death is an invitation for growth”.
Acknowledgement The authors thank the Dean, K.E.M. Hospital and Seth G.S. Medical College, Bombay, for permitting work in this hospital
and allowing publication of this paper. The authors also thank the staff of the General Medical Ward attached to Dr. (Miss)
Acharya's Unit and Department of Nephrology, K.E.M. Hospital, for their cooperation. References AITKEN- SWAN, J. & EASSON E.C. (1959),
Reactions of cancer patients on being told their diagnosis, British Medical Journal, 1,779-783. BEILEN, R. (1981), Social functions of denial of death, Omega, 12(1), 25-25. BRUHN, J.G., PAREDES, A., ADSETT,
C.A. & WOLF, S. (1974), Psychological predictors of sudden death in myocardial
infarction, Journal of Psychosomatic Residency, 18, 187-191. CASSEM, N.H. & STEWART, R.S. (1975),
Management and Care of the Dying Patient, International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 6, 293-304.
CRAMOND, W.A. (1970), Psychotherapy of the Dying Patient, British Medical Journal,
Aug.15, 3, 389-393. GOLDBERG, I.K., MAUTZ, S., & KUTSCHER,
A.H. (eds.) (1973), Psychopharmacological Agents for the Terminally III and Bereaved. New York: Columbia University Press.
HACKETI, T.P. & CASSEM, N.H. (1972),
Patients facing sudden cardiac death. In L.B. Schoenberg, A.C. Car, D. Peretz and A.H. Kutscher (eds.), Psychosocial Aspects
of Terminal Care, New Columbia University Press, 47-56. HACKETI. T.P. & WEISMAN. A.D. (1962),
The Treatment of the Dying. Current Psychiatric Therapies, 2. 121-126. HICKS, W. & DANIELS. R.S. (1968). The
Dying Patient. his physician and the psychiatric consultant, Psychosomatics, 9, 47-52. HINTON. J. (1972), Psychiatric consultation in fatal illness. Proceedings of the
Royal Society of Medicine, 65, 1035-38. JOSEPH,
F. (1962), Transference and counter-transference in the case of a dying patient, Psychoanalytical
Review, 49, 21-34. KAMATH, M. V. (1978), Philosophy of Death and Dying. Pensylvania,
Himalaya International Institute of Yoga Science & Philosophy. KOTHARI, M.L. & MEHTA, L.A. (1986), Death. New Perspective of the phenomena of Disease and Dying. London: Marion Boyers. KRANT, M. J. (1981), Hospice Philosophy in late stage cancer
care, Joumal of the American Medical Association, 245, 1061-1062. KUBLER ROSS, E. (1969), On Death and Dying. New York: Macmillan. KUBLER ROSS, E. (1974), Questions and Answers on Death and Dying,
New York: Collier Books. KUBLER ROSS, E. (1975), Death - the final stage of Growth. Eagle Cliffs, New Jersey:
Prentice-Hall.
LESHAN, L.L. & LESHAN, E. (1961), Psychotherapy and the patients with a limited
life span, Psychiatry, 24, 318-323. LEVlNSON, P. (1972), On Sudden death, Psychiarry,
35,160-173. MOODY, RA. (Jr.), (1975), Life After Death. Atlanta: Mocking bird books. OKEN, D. (1961), What to tell cancer patients; a study of medical
attitude, Journal of the American Medical Association, 175, 1120-1128. SABOM, M.B., (1982), Recollections of Death: A Medical Investigation,
New York: Harper and Row. SAUNDERS, C. (1969), The moment of truth; care of the dying
person, In: P.L. Cleveland (Ed.) Death and Dying. Case Western Reserve University Press, 49-78. STEDEFORD, A. (1979), Psychotherapy of the Dying Patient, British Journal of Psychiatry, 135, 7-14. WEISMAN, A.D. (1967), The patient with a fatal illness
- to tell or not to tell, Journal of the America Medical Association, 201, 646-648. WHITMAN, H.H. & LUKES, S.L (1975), Behaviour Modification
for terminally ill patients, American Journal of Nursing, 75, 98-101.
|
||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
|
Enter supporting content here
|