Ajai R. Singh MD

Ajai R. Singh MD | Media Links | Music Streaming Sites for Album 'Khushnuma Raat Hai' | Online Availability of Album, 'Khushnuma Raat Hai' | Editor, Mens Sana Monographs [2003- ] | At PubMed, PMC, NLM, OCLC etc | Mens Sana Monographs at NLM, PubMed, PMC and IndMed | Short Bio [For talks] | Monographs, Book and Book Chapters | Lectures, Awards, Orations | Music, 'Khushnuma raat hai', Mirza Ghalib and YouTube Links | Music: About the artiste | Poetry | Life and Influences | Contact

Death, Dying And Near Death Experience

Indian Journal of Psychiatry, July 1988, 30(3), p299-306
 
 
DEATH, DYING AND NEAR DEATH EXPERIENCE

Preliminary report on surveying the need and developing the method

                       

 

AJAI R. SINGHl

V.N. BAGADIA2

P.V. PRADHAN3

V.N. ACHARYA4

 

SUMMARY

Psychology of deaths due to acute medical emergencies is under-researched. Most studies till now have concentrated on extended-death situations like malignancy. This open pilot study of twenty-five patients examines the psychological state of patients during a life threatening acute medical illness (Group A, ten pa­tients) and of those who survive such an experience (Group B, fifteen patients). The study finds psychological exploration both possible and necessary if carried out in a discreet manner. Salient features of the interview technique are discussed. The study finds out whether patients are aware of the possibility of terminality. The psychological disturbances manifest and nature of care expected are also discussed. Near Death Experiences of those who acknowledge their occurrence are reported. Some nuances of thanatological research are high-lighted: What are the abilities needed in an interviewer? Can such exploration increase psychological distress in a patient already prone to it because of serious medical sickness? What impact such research can have on the interviewer himself? The paper answers some of these common questions while developing the method of thanatological study in acute medical death-situations.

 

…………………………………………………………………..

1.   Senior Research Fellow, W.H.O. Collaborating Center in psychopharmacology

2.   Prof. Emeritus in psychiatry, Seth G.S. Medical College and K.E.M. Hospital. Head, W.H.O. Collaborating Centre in Psychopharmacology. Department of Psychiatry, K.E.M. Hospital

3.   Hon. Associate Prof. of Psychiatry. Seth GS Medical College and K.E.M. Hospital. Bombay

4.   Head, Department of Medicine & Nephrology, and Prof. of Medicine. Seth G.S. Medical College and K.E.M. Hospital. Bombay

*Bombay Psychiatric Society Silver Jubilee Year Award winning paper. Jan. 1988, at XXXX Annual Confer­ence of IPS. Also President Award winning paper at XVIIl Annual Conference of IPS. West Zone, Sept. 1987.

 

 

Introduction

Scientific enquiry into death is a rela­tively recent phenomenon. The psychiat­rist's role in the terminal patient traces its history to the last three quarters of a cen­tury.

Treatment of the dying keeping his psychological needs in mind gained atten­tion (HacKen and Weisman 1962) as did Hospice care (Krant 1981). Use of treat­ment modalities in terminal patients like psychotherapy (Leshan and Leshan 1961, Cramond 1970, Stedeford 1979), Be­haviour Therapy (Whitman and Lukes 1975) and psychopharmacologic agents (Goldberg et al. 1973) were studied. Counter-transference attitudes of health personnel dealing with the dying (Joseph 1962, Hicks and Daniels 1968) as well as when, what and how to disclose (Ait­kenSwan and Easson 1959; Oken 1961, Weisman 1967) occupied the attention of other researches. Kubler-Ross’ epoch making work (1969,1974,1975) served as a launching pad, as though, for a global up­surge in thanotological research. Near Death Experiences or NDEs also occupied the attention of quite a few others (Moody 1975, Sabom 1982).

Most of the studies (a number of them cannot be quoted for exigencies of space) have evaluated dying as a process ex­tended over a period of time, for example, following the diagnosis of malignancy through the resultant morbidity to even­tual death. Deaths due to acute life ­threatening medical emergencies has es­caped the attention of researchers, barring a few, like Levinson (1972) who discussed psychological state of patients facing sud­den death, Hackett & Cassem (1972) who examined the psychology of cardiac arrest, and Bruhn et al. (1974) who evaluated psychological predictors of sudden death in cardiac patients. This, however, is a miniscule minority, probably for pragma­tic reasons. The equation needs to be set right. Moreover death and dying has es­caped the attention of researchers in India, in this age of the thanatologic imperative (Kothari and Mehta 1986) and in a country which not without reason can boast of a great philosophical tradition on the sub­ject, right from the Vedas to Sri Au­robindo (Kamath 1978). The present ef­fort may be considered one in the direction of setting the equation right.

Aims of the Study

1.  Is it possible to study the psychology of death and dying in a general medical set-up?

2.   Whether there is a need to do so?

3.   How does one go about it?

4.   Are psychiatric disturbances manifest by such patients?

5.   Are Near Death Experiences (NDEs) reported in those who recover?

6.   What are the needs and expectations of such patients?

Material and Methods

A total of 25 patients (Group A, 10, and, Group B, 15) from the General Medical Ward administering both emergency and ongoing care in a teaching general hos­pital in Greater Bombay were included in this open pilot study. Their ages ranged from 12 to 61 years. The sample was made up predominantly by the age groups 16-30 years (56%) and 31-45 years (28%).

Group A (10 patients - 7 male and, 3 female) was seen when acutely sick medi­cally and emergency life-sustaining proce­dures were being administered. Group A thus consists of seriously sick patients with a definite mortality risk.

Group B (15 patients - 9 male, 6 female) consisted of patients who had been through a life threatening medical illness and were now in the recovery phase. They came from either the ICCU or Intensive Nephrology Care Unit.

Interview Technique

All patients were first screened on the weekly rounds with the Physician-In-Charge and the resident staff. They were then interviewed by one of the authors having post-graduate qualification in psychiatry (ARS). The Mental Status Examination of all patients was done. The interview schedule was semi-structured, being conducted in 1-6 sessions. Open-ended questions were usually asked. Rarely were leading questions used be­cause of their psychologically disturbing potential. This, rather than asking 'Do you think your sickness can result in something serious? May be...death?' the question put was, 'What do you feel can be the out­come of your present sickness?'

If the question was parried, one or two discreet questions put were, 'You know, being in a hospital with lots of doc­tors and nurses around and so many tubes being used on a person can cause anxiety. How does that affect you?' If still there was no answer, a comment was made like, 'It appears you are satisfied with what is being done for you. We are happy you are confi­dent of a smooth recovery?' From either of the comments, the patient broke his initial reserve and became communicative.

The interview itself had to be discreet because of the sensitive nature of the sub­ject tackled. Also there was the continuous need to avoid arousing undue anxiety in patients, already over-burdened with the distress consequent to physical morbidity, and even in relatives, and the ward-staff. No misplaced zeal need be used in any psychological investigation that adversely affects the emotional equilibrium of the seriously sick and their relatives. A firm commitment to this effect from the psychiatric side is a must, whether asked for or not. This tends to keep over-en­thusiasm in check and zealousness tem­pered, while the genuine keenness of re­search is never compromised.

 

Results and Discussion

I. The Sample: Most Group A pa­tients developed their sickness less than three months prior to hospitalization (60%) while Group B was represented, 73 %, by those who developed their sick­ness more than three months prior to hos­pitalization. Group A, thus, represented patients who had fallen sick relatively re­cently (average duration 10 days) while Group B consisted of those who had symptoms for a longer period (average du­ration 100 days). Majority of Group A patients were seen 24 hours - 14 days of de­veloping acute symptoms (80%). Group B patients were seen 8 days - 30 days follow­ing recovery of the acute episode (87%).

Thus the acutely sick were seen during their acute phase and the recovering pa­tient in a reasonable time following the acute phase and during their active recov­ery and rehabilitative period. The sample may, therefore, be considered fairly rep­resentative of the phenomenon studied.

Table 1

Diagnostic Break-up, Group A (N = 10)

Diagnosis

Expired

Recovered

Total

Fulminant Pneumonia

2

1

3

Advanced Tuberculosis

1

1

2

Hepatic Encephalopathy

2

0

2

Acute Rental failure

1

2

3

 

6

4

10

Table 2

Diagnostic Break-up, Group B (N = 15)

Diagnosis

Male

Female

Total

Myocardial infarction and/or Ischaemia

5

3

8

Acute Rental failure in a patient with chronic renal failure

3

4

7

 

8

7

15

Comment: All patients needed emergency resuscita­tion measures and had survived.

 

II. Diagnosis: Table 1 gives the diag­nostic break-up in Group A. The common life threatening sicknesses were Fulminant Pneumonia, Advanced Tuberculosis, Hepatic Encephalopathy and Acute Renal Failure. Six of these patients expired and four recovered.

 

Table 2 shows the diagnostic break-up in Group B. Myocardial Infarction and/or Ischaemia, and Acute Renal Failure in patients with Chronic Renal Failure are represented here. The diagnoses represent acute life threatening medical situations commonly encountered in a general medi­cal ward.

 

III. Mental Status Examination

Table 1

Group A: Mental Status Examination (N = 10)

 

Item

No.

%

Clouding of Consciousness

Delirium

Coma

Anxiety Symptoms

Paranoid ideation

Depressive Symptoms

Conversions Symptoms

Obsessive Compulsive Symptoms

10

6

5

4

3

2

1

1

100

60

50

40

30

20

10

10

 

Note: More than one symptom was often present in the same patient.

 

Table 3 shows Mental Status Examination (MSE) results in Group A. Clouding of consciousness was present in all patients at some time or other. But this did not pre­clude further MSE. 50% went with coma and 60% had episodes of delirium, either preceding or succeeding the MSE. Lack of communicativeness (3 pts.), lack of per­sonal hygiene (4 pts.) and incontinence (2 pts.) were the usual accompaniments of disturbed consciousness.

 

Terror with florid paranoid ideation (3 pts.); hallucinations (visual 2 pts; audit­ory 1 pt.); depressed mood (2 pts.); crying spells, guilt feelings, suicidal ideation (1 pt.); anxiety (4 pts.); somatic preoccupa­tion (5 pts.); conversion symptoms (1 pt.); and compulsive symptoms (1 pt.) were also seen.

 

Orientation to time, place, or person was disturbed in seven out of ten patients. Memory appeared disturbed in five, immediate (5 pts.), recent (5 pts.) and past (2 pts.). Intelligence was affected in two pa­tients.

 

Anxiety, Paranoid ideation, Depre­ssed mood, Conversion symptoms and Ob­sessive compulsive symptoms were present and seemed to be exacerbation of basic personality characteristics.

 

Group A, thus, consisted of acute medical emergencies who progressively lapsed into disorganized states of the mind. This is especially true of those who subsequently expired (60%). These pa­tients were reasonably young and the dura­tion of morbidity was quite short. Thus, acute medical emergencies may leave little time for psychological understanding or for the relatives to cope with the probabil­ity of death in a family. It differs consider­ably from an extended death situation like malignancy. Follow up of close relatives of such patients needs further study. Impor­tant areas could be the repercussions of lack of time for psychological rehabilita­tion, or the time to say good-bye. Also worth exploration would be how patients and their relatives compress the stages of dying in the short time at their disposal, how they succeed and where they fail, and the squeal of either.

 

Table 4 shows mental status examina­tion in Group B. Almost all patients were eager to establish communication. Only 3 patients showed mild clouding of con­sciousness. No perceptual or cognitive dis­turbances were noted; neither were Insight or judgment impaired.

 

 

 

Table 4

Group B: Mental Status Examination (N = 15)

Item

No.

%

Clouding of Consciousness (mild)

Anxiety Symptoms

Depressive Symptoms

Indifference

Psychologically Normal

3

8

3

2

2

20

53

20

13

13

 

Note: Symptoms overlap in some patients.

 

Anxiety symptoms were manifest m eight patients of Group B. They were ap­prehensive about the sickness, were afraid it was severe and would seriously cripple their day to day activities (although the medical opinion warranted such a conclu­sion in only four such cases). They com­plained of difficulty in getting sleep (8 pts.), tremulousness (7pts.), palpitations (6 pts.), and fear of fresh acute episode of their sickness. They occasionally showed clinging behaviour (4 pts.) and asked for repeated reassurance from medical staff and relatives (6 pts.). They had occasional spell of crying (3 pts.) with sulking behaviour (4 pts.) and irritability (6 pts.). Some complained of fleeting suicidal thought (2 pts.) All patients were anxious about the future welfare of their family members.

Three patients showed depressed mood. They felt life was not worth living (3 pts.), it would be better to die (3 pts.), and after-life would be better (2 pts.). They felt guilty of insufficient provision for family members (2pts.), prayed for a miracle to cure their sickness (2 pts.), prayed that life be prolonged to carry out pressing business and family duties (2 pts.).

Two patients appeared indifferent to the severity of their sickness. They main­tained a resigned attitude, appeared to take excessive interest in religion and had faith of tiding over their present crisis. They however also complained of petty in­consistencies in the behaviour of relatives and medical staff. Two patients appeared to take the experience with minimal of psychological symptoms. Group B pa­tients showed a keen desire to talk and unburden themselves, but this need was not met by either the staff or the relatives.

IV. Psychiatric Disturbances: Anxi­ety was the predominant symptom in both Groups A and B (Table 3 and 4). This is understandable due to an experience which is felt so near death. Lot of this anx­iety centered around the future course and outcome of the illness which almost no patient knew for sure. This is probably due to an honest doubt in the physicians' mind whether such knowledge can have its own psychological repercussions. Anxiety (53%) and Depressive (20%) were seen as clinically diagnosable disease entities in Group B patients (Table 3).

Terror with paranoid ideation where manifest (Table 3) involved persecutory delusions about relatives and medical staff. Those patients who showed no ab­normality (Table 4) seemed to have both an element of faith and awareness of prac­ticalities. They had had fleeting anxiety and depressive symptoms. They felt grate­ful for the support of at least one significant relative, of religious scriptures, and the ward-staff in their crisis.

V. Awareness of Terminality: 60% patients in both Group A and B were aware of the possibility of terminality (Table 5). The similarity in both groups is striking and needs further study. As at pre­sent, it appeared more a manifestation of sickness severity rather than a premoni­tion.

 

Table 5

Aware of Possibility of Terminality in both Groups A and B (N = 25)

Item

Group A (N=10)        GROUP B (N=15)     TOTAL

 

NO.

%

NO.

%

NO.

%

Aware of possibility of trainability

6

60

9

60

15

60

Unaware of possibility of Terminality

4

40

6

40

10

40

 

                   Note :- 60% of both Groups A and B were aware of the possibility of terminality.

 

VI. Near Death Experience NDE): Table 6 shows NDEs. 10 patients out of 19 replied in the affirmative when questioned for it. Their experiences are mainly ‘Intense Darkness’ (3 patients) and ‘Total Silence’ (3 patients). Other items were bright lights, receding into the background, vision of Christ and vision of Yama.

 

Table 6

Near Death Experience (NDE)

(N=10, Group A-2, Group B 8)

Item

NO.

%

Feeling of intense darkness

Feeling of total silence

Feeling of bright light and exploding firecrackers

Feeling of receding into the background to become a dot

Vision of tall grey haired bearded man with flowing robes standing with open arms (identification as Christ)

Vision of a burly individual riding a bull

(Identified as Yama deva)

3

3

1

1

 

1

 

1

30

30

-*

-

 

-

 

-

 

*Denotes irrelevance of % statistics here because of small sample.

 

VII. Nature of Care Asked for: Almost all patients asked for prompt relief from pain and discomfort (84%) and distress free digestive and evacuative functioning (84%). They wanted reassurance that they would improve (60%) and asked for a painless end if it became inevitable (36%). They liked cheerfulness in medical staff (80%), appreciated competence in symptom relief (88%) and singled out individual staff members for accolades (7%) and brick bats (56%). They disapproved of anger in staff members (72%), neglect (52%), evasive answers (405) or false reassurances (32%). These items show that patients were keenly aware of what when on in their environment.

 

It was not always that detailed talk or reassurance was asked for. Often patients appreciated acts like a warm greeting, a gentle touch of comfort, a helping hand in getting up from bed, an allowing of the doctor's hand to be clasped, and an opportun­ity to cry or touch the feet. A regular, brief, cheerful visit was better appreciated than a prolonged, irregular one. Patients disliked caring persons who avoided eye­ contact either while talking to them or even while passing them by.

In most cases that survived, patients were grateful to recount the heroic mea­sures carried out. But many were equally expectant that care of a significant inten­sity would still be continued.

A word or two about patients' over­zealous methods at rehabilitation also re­veals another aspect of the nature of care these patients need. This applies particu­larly to patients physically restricted dur­ing their rehabilitation, specially the myocardial infarction patients of Group B. While on Occupational Therapy, they sometimes over-stretched themselves when unobserved.

Sexual needs were again a neglected area of counseling. At/east two patients of infarction confided they masturbated in hospital without knowledge of the medical staff. They were too embarrassed to ask for advice. A straight forward approach to counseling about sex or masturbation in recuperating infarct patients is therefore/ necessary. Although the number is small (i.e. 3 out of 9 cardiac patients), these are sensitive areas. Patients may be unable to reveal them, yet it may he the need of quite a few. Further study of factors which help and/or hinder rehabilitation and how the caring team could by simple and straight forward explanation help in dispelling doubts and fears would be worthwhile.

VIII. Comments on the Interview Session & Techniques: The authors found that initial resistance to being interviewed was born out of a great need to deny. Any denial, by both patients and relatives, seems to serve an importance social function (Beilen, 1981). It maintains the semblance of a relationship based on hope and courage. Where denial was an important defense, the interview was conducted in short sessions, in as less glum a manner as possible. The man­ner of the interview was such as to instil cor­diality. It involved, amongst other things, listening to the difficulties of being in a gen­eral medical ward. It also meant passing on relevant information to the ward staff for necessary action. The interviewer thus be­came something of a liaison worker here.

All of us have a rather strong inner re­sistance to letting dying persons say what's on their minds (Cassem and Stewart 1975), because it increases our own death-anxiety in turn. This applies to medical men as well as relatives. But practically every empiri­cal study in this field has emphasised the ability to listen over the ability to say some­thing (ibid). The patient does not only want answers to his questions. He often wants a receptive, knowing and caring situ­ation. In the bargain, he is ready to offer the researchers any number of enlighten­ing answers to his questions. Probably, Saunders summed it up beautifully when she said, "The real question is not, 'What do you tell your patients?' but rather, 'what do you let your patients tell you?" (Saunders 1969).

It is difficult to resist the experience of being carried away by certain emotions in thanatological research. This is the univer­sal experience of most such researchers. This again is a reality every researcher in this field must be ready to accept. In fact if he does not do so, it is only a measure of his denial. or lack of awareness.

As Kubler-Ross (1975) so succinctly put it. "Each encounter with death is an in­vitation for growth”.

 

Acknowledgement

 

The authors thank the Dean, K.E.M. Hospital and Seth G.S. Medical College, Bombay, for permitting work in this hospi­tal and allowing publication of this paper. The authors also thank the staff of the General Medical Ward attached to Dr. (Miss) Acharya's Unit and Department of Nephrology, K.E.M. Hospital, for their cooperation.

 

References

AITKEN- SWAN, J. & EASSON E.C. (1959), Reactions of cancer patients on being told their diagnosis, British Medical Journal, 1,779-783.

BEILEN, R. (1981), Social functions of denial of death, Omega, 12(1), 25-25.

BRUHN, J.G., PAREDES, A., ADSETT, C.A. & WOLF, S. (1974), Psychological predictors of sudden death in myocardial infarction, Journal of Psychosomatic Residency, 18, 187-191.

CASSEM, N.H. & STEWART, R.S. (1975), Man­agement and Care of the Dying Patient, Interna­tional Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 6, 293­-304.

CRAMOND, W.A. (1970), Psychotherapy of the Dying Patient, British Medical Journal, Aug.15, 3, 389-393.

GOLDBERG, I.K., MAUTZ, S., & KUTSCHER, A.H. (eds.) (1973), Psychopharmacological Agents for the Terminally III and Bereaved. New York: Columbia University Press.

HACKETI, T.P. & CASSEM, N.H. (1972), Patients facing sudden cardiac death. In L.B. Schoenberg, A.C. Car, D. Peretz and A.H. Kutscher (eds.), Psychosocial Aspects of Termi­nal Care, New Columbia University Press, 47-56.

HACKETI. T.P. & WEISMAN. A.D. (1962), The Treatment of the Dying. Current Psychiatric Therapies, 2. 121-126.

HICKS, W. & DANIELS. R.S. (1968). The Dying Patient. his physician and the psychiatric consultant, Psychosomatics, 9, 47-52.

HINTON. J. (1972), Psychiatric consultation in fatal illness. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine, 65, 1035-38.

JOSEPH, F. (1962), Transference and counter-transference in the case of a dying patient, Psychoanalytical Review, 49, 21­-34.

KAMATH, M. V. (1978), Philosophy of Death and Dying. Pensylvania, Himalaya International In­stitute of Yoga Science & Philosophy.

KOTHARI, M.L. & MEHTA, L.A. (1986), Death.  New Perspective of the phenomena of Disease and Dying. London: Marion Boyers.

KRANT, M. J. (1981), Hospice Philosophy in late stage cancer care, Joumal of the American Medi­cal Association, 245, 1061-1062.

KUBLER ROSS, E. (1969), On Death and Dying. New York: Macmillan.

KUBLER ROSS, E. (1974), Questions and Answers on Death and Dying, New York: Collier Books.

KUBLER ROSS, E. (1975), Death - the final stage of Growth. Eagle Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.

                 LESHAN, L.L. & LESHAN, E. (1961), Psychotherapy and the patients with a limited life span, Psychiatry, 24, 318-323.

LEVlNSON, P. (1972), On Sudden death, Psychiarry, 35,160-173.

MOODY, RA. (Jr.), (1975), Life After Death. At­lanta: Mocking bird books.

OKEN, D. (1961), What to tell cancer patients; a study of medical attitude, Journal of the Ameri­can Medical Association, 175, 1120-1128.

SABOM, M.B., (1982), Recollections of Death: A Medical Investigation, New York: Harper and Row.

SAUNDERS, C. (1969), The moment of truth; care of the dying person, In: P.L. Cleveland (Ed.) Death and Dying. Case Western Reserve Uni­versity Press, 49-78.

STEDEFORD, A. (1979), Psychotherapy of the Dying Patient, British Journal of Psychiatry, 135, 7-14.

WEISMAN, A.D. (1967), The patient with a fatal ill­ness - to tell or not to tell, Journal of the America Medical Association, 201, 646-648.

WHITMAN, H.H. & LUKES, S.L (1975), Behaviour Modification for terminally ill pa­tients, American Journal of Nursing, 75, 98­-101.

 

 

<<<Previous

Next>>>

Enter content here

Enter supporting content here